The following article (including the pic of me in my PhD office) originally appeared in
's "New Voices" section in 2011. It is reprinted here with permission.
Allen, A.P. (2011). “The Zoo of the New”: Organisational innovation in
universities and the psychology of creativity. The Psychologist, 24(11),
862-863.
Despite the image of the university as a hub of learning and imagination,
many of its inhabitants feel that they are failing to use their creativity to
bring about something useful. They may look at their research and see the same
technique being applied to trivially differing questions. Others read through a
syllabus, see that it’s not engaging and yet do not know what to do with it.
And yet pressure to be more innovative seems to be everywhere; for example, the
“New technology, innovation and skills investment” webpage of the Economic and
Social Research Council (ESRC, 2009). They note that much of the UK’s value
added is coming from high-tech manufacturing and, more relevant for
psychologists, knowledge-intensive services. But what might help or hinder organisational
innovation? And in whose interest is it that universities become more
innovative?
Creativity and/versus organisational innovation
For this discussion, I will refer to organisational
innovation(s) (OI) as creativity that enhances income and/or organisational
efficiency. Simonton (1988) has argued that creativity requires social
persuasion. Although such a strong view is obviously controversial,
dissemination of an idea is a typical part of the creative process for many
creative people. Most theorists of creativity (though not all; c.f. Weisberg,
2006) suggest that the main criteria for creativity are novelty and usefulness.
A creative thought that becomes an OI can be thought of not only as having been
disseminated, but as also having passed the test of usefulness.
Sharing an idea is all well and good, but some may
baulk at the idea of a creative idea needing to prove itself in the workplace
or marketplace. Nonetheless, creative ideas need to overcome organisational
barriers in order to manifest themselves as innovation (c.f. De Dreu, Nijstad, Bechtoldt, & Baas,
2011). I would argue that, for example, turning an idea into a marketable
product is sufficient for meeting the usefulness criterion, but not necessary. Figuring
out how to make an idea appeal to others can contribute to the creative
process. Indeed, Boden (e.g. 2004) has argued that constraints are what make
creativity possible. Constraints placed on thinking create a conceptual map of
creative possibilities; breakthroughs can come by explorations of these
conceptual spaces (and at the boundaries, where the rules can be bent). Turning
one’s idea into OI will create new constraints (e.g. “laypeople” need to understand
this thing in the way that I do) and these can help to refine one’s ideas.
OI and creativity in psychology
As scholars of the mind we should be able to generate useful ideas and
apply knowledge about creativity to our own work. To take an example of
relevant research, surveys of R & D employees has found that time pressure hinders
creative outcomes, but only in organisations with a strong innovation climate
(Hsu & Fan, 2010). This could have clear implications for universities that
have committed themselves to fostering an innovative climate (although applying
this finding may not always be easy). A
series of experiments have indicated that thinking on behalf of others tends to
lead to more creative thinking (Polman & Emich, 2011). Asking colleagues
for their opinion may thus lead to more original work if one takes their advice.
Psychological research on creativity is obviously useful, but broader
psychological theories of creativity can also contribute to the debate. Both
Boden (2004) and Weisberg (2006) have argued that ordinary thought processes
are sufficient for the creation of creative ideas. The implication is that most
people have the potential to develop OI – someone’s creative ability shouldn’t
be written off just he/she does a particular type of work.
But if anyone can be creative, why aren’t we all coming up with amazing ideas?
Both Boden and Weisberg have also argued that one must spend a long period of
time developing expertise in a field before one can make a major creative
contribution. Perhaps some who find themselves frequently changing jobs may
never make a major breakthrough as a result. However, I don’t believe that
geographical mobility should be seen as an impediment to creativity. Consider
the finding that people who have spent time living abroad tend to perform
better at creative thinking tasks (Maddux & Galinsky, 2009).
One barrier to problem solving is functional fixedness
(thinking about an object in terms of its typical function, when a solution
requires one to think about it in a different way) (Duncker, 1945). It can be
difficult to overcome: Jansson and Smith (1991) demonstrated that engineers and
engineering students included a bad aspect of a prototype when asked to create
a new design, even though they were instructed to avoid this bad aspect. One
advantage of moving to a new lab or department is that one’s “fresh pair of
eyes” may use a novel approach to a research question or invention that’s under
development. Even if new members of staff do not spot an idea by themselves,
their relatively naïve questions may inspire the more experienced members of
staff to overcome a functional fixation that had previously trapped them.
It is probably uncontroversial to assert that
academics have to work hard to bring a fully-fledged creative project online.
But does this imply that researching a number of different topics could undermine
creative potential? Not necessarily. Useful
insights may often strike due to opportunistic assimilation (c.f. Seifert et
al., 1995). This is where one encounters useful concepts that help to further
an idea when one is not in the midst of trying to develop the idea. For
example, a group of behavioural scientists who undertake to learn more about
the organisation of the brain may, during this process of learning, get the
inspiration for a new piece of experimental equipment for their animal lab.
However, opportunistic assimilation is more likely to occur when a problem has
been deeply considered, although not fully resolved. Consequently, the more one
has considered an issue, the easier it is for opportunistic assimilation to
occur. It seems one simply has to juggle working hard at a major project while
staying abreast of other developments.
Many psychologists have already shown opportunistic assimilation by involving
themselves in the emergence of multidisciplinary areas (cognitive science and
behavioural economics are fecund examples). One example of how psychology can
interact with other disciplines is the tension between organisational
psychology and economics over how to handle job security (Alsop, 1996). Organisational
psychology has found that psychological security can foster innovation (c.f.
West, 2000, for a discussion), while many economists encourage a push for
increasing labour market flexibility. Alsop suggests that enhancing
employability (as opposed to job security) may help to ease this tension
somewhat. You may or may not agree with this proposed solution (bearing in mind
the above point about frequent job changes). Nonetheless, questions like these
have clear social relevance…the upshot is that the dialogue between psychology
and other disciplines should help universities to increase their influence.
Left, right, left, right
Some may perceive the drive towards OI in academia (rather than creativity
in general) as a ham-fisted attempt to turn university staff into (1) lapdogs
for industry, rather than its collaborators and/or (2) administrators that are
expected to make their institution run more smoothly instead of pursuing their
intellectual ideas. If either of these concerns is true, the creativity of
university staff may be stifled.
The first anxiety may turn to panic with the funding for “blue skies”
research beginning to feel the pinch under the latest UK government – with this push
coming in a context where people have already suggested that public/private
collaborations may not be in the public interest (James, 2002). Where such
collaborations go sour, the second anxiety may be vindicated if university
researchers find themselves spending their time organising the collaboration
instead of staying at the coalface of research. Although there is an argument that innovation only needs
to be original for the organisation implementing it (e.g. De Dreu et al.,
2011), my own fear is that the label “Innovation” could undermine
radical, groundbreaking creativity in research – is there still anything original about universities assisting in
clinical trials for pharmaceutical companies?
But let’s not forget the genuine innovations of both university staff and
students. Some computer scientists at a university I previously attended (who
went on to set up a successful business) grumbled that a sufficient support
structure was not there for them to develop patents for technologies emerging
from their work. However, there are now many organisations helping graduates to
establish spin-off companies based on their ideas. Many universities have
innovation centres - ideally, businesses and universities can collaborate here,
although again people will fret that more open-ended research (which,
ironically, can lead to the most lucrative ideas) could be undermined.
Much of the creativity of psychologists in universities does not
translate into obvious OI. Let’s say one puts a newly-developed questionnaire
online for free. If many other researchers/clinicians use it, then clearly this
is a successful idea, whether or not it is actually making anyone rich or helping
one’s own university to top some league table. It should not surprise us that
creativity is not always “cashed in”; research (albeit with children) has
indicated that more creative ideas are driven by intrinsic motivation (i.e.
motivation that comes from within oneself), rather than extrinsic motivation
(i.e. motivation that comes from others) (Amabile, 1982). However, recent research with
undergraduate students has indicated that while intrinsic motivation should be
important for radical creativity, extrinsic motivation drives more incremental,
step-by-step creativity (Gilson & Madjar, 2011). Nonetheless, the point
stands that the most creative ideas may come about when people are allowed to
pursue their passion.
An invitation
Realistically, “hard science” departments will probably continue to
dominate in terms of technological innovation. But psychology departments, and
certainly their members with an interest in positive psychology, creativity, or
problem solving, can play a key role in turning their universities into
environments that encourage creativity. At a broad level, here are some of the
challenges we can take up:
·
How can the cognitive and social psychology of
creative thinking be applied to research, invention and teaching?
·
How can we involve students to a greater extent
in the learning and research process?
·
How can we develop administrative systems that
structure and organise our research without excessively slowing it down?
Clearly, answering some of these questions will be a lot easier if we
work with administrative staff and researchers in education and sociology.
Fostering a general potential for creativity in universities may not be
as financially rewarding as coming up with the latest industrial advance. However,
I hope that all this does not just sound like an appeal to pure altruism; to
paraphrase Jack Nicholson in The Departed,
your job is most rewarding when you are not a product of your work environment,
but rather your work environment is a product of you.
References:
Alsop, A. (1996). Innovation and research: Themes and research funding. European Journal of Work and Organisational
Psychology, 5(1), 149-153.
Amabile, T. M.
(1982). Social psychology of creativity: A consensual assessment technique. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 43(5), 997-1013.
Boden, M.A. (2004). The creative
mind: Myths and mechanisms (2nd Ed). London: Routledge.
De Dreu, C.K., Nijstad,
B. A., Bechtoldt, M.N., & Baas, M. (2011). Group creativity and
innovation: A motivated information processing perspective. Psychology of
Creativity, Aesthetics and the Arts, 5(1), 81-89.
Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological monographs. 68(5,
whole no. 270).
Gilson, L.L., Madjar, N.
(2011). Radical and incremental creativity: Antecedents and processes. Psychology
of Creativity, Aesthetics and the Arts, 5(1), 21-28.
Hsu, M.L.A. & Fan, H. (2010). Organisational innovation climate and
creative outcomes: Exploring the moderating effect of time pressure. Creativity Research Journal, 22(4), 378-386.
James, J.E. (2002). “Third-party” threats to research integrity in
public-private partnerships. Addiction, 97, 1251-1255.
Jansson, D.G. & Smith, S.M. (1991). Design fixation. Design studies, 12(1), 3-11.
Maddux, W. & Galinsky, A. (2009). Cultural borders and mental
barriers: The relationship between living abroad and creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
95(5), 1047-1061.
Polman & Emich, (2011). Decisions for others are more creative than
decisions for the self. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin. 37(4),
492-501.
Seifert, C.M., Meyer, D.E., Davidson, N., Patalano, A.L., Yaniv, I. (1995). Demystification of cognitive insight:
Opportunistic assimilation and the prepared-mind perspective. In R.J. Sternberg
& J.E. Davidson (Eds.) The nature of
insight. (pp. 65-124). Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Simonton, D.K. (1988). Creativity, leadership and chance. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.) The nature of creativity (pp. 386-426). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Weisberg, R.W. (2006). Creativity: Understanding innovation in
problem solving, science, invention and the arts. Hoboken, NJ:
John Wiley & Sons.
West, M.A. (2000). Creativity and innovation at work. The
Psychologist, 13(9), 460-464.